Window on the Clearwater
Traditional news Today's technology

Advertisement

8-9-22

healthtalk.JPG - 5177 Bytes
htsponsors2.PNG - 6545 Bytes

Study points to priming the brain as an aid to learning

  • After people were incidentally exposed to new objects, they learned to categorize them faster when explicitly taught to recognize them later.
  • These study results are some of the first to show that the human brain is made ready to learn by incidental exposure to new things.

The human brain is skilled at categorizing. People can quickly recognize a new variation of something they've seen before, like a dog, a chair, a jacket, or a lamp. We do this even when we've received very little explicit teaching about what distinguishes such categories.

How the brain builds this category knowledge hasn't been well understood. Drs. Layla Unger and Vladimir Sloutsky from Ohio State University designed a set of experiments to learn how incidental exposure to new things shapes later learning about categories.

The experiments had two phases: an exposure phase and an explicit learning phase. In both phases, participants saw images of imaginary creatures with body parts such as heads, tails, and antennae in various shapes and colors.

In the exposure phase, volunteers played a video game in which a creature appeared in the center, disappeared, and then jumped to the right or left. Participants had to indicate which way the creature jumped, but weren't told anything about the creatures. Some participants saw creatures made up of random combinations of body parts. Others saw creatures that belonged to different categories, sharing body parts of specific shapes and colors.

For some who saw creatures that belonged to categories, creatures from the same category had many body parts in common. These are called dense categories. For others, the creatures belonged to sparse categories. That is, they had only a few body parts in common, such as having only the same head and tail color. In both cases, creatures in different categories jumped in different directions. This meant that, if participants started to recognize the categories on their own, they could perform the game faster. This let researchers track whether implicit learning had occurred.

After playing this game, participants were told that the creatures belonged to two categories, called "flurps" and "jalets," and received explicit training on how to tell them apart. The researchers then measured how incidental exposure to the categories affected their explicit learning. Results of the study, which was funded by National Institute of Health, were published on May 26 in Psychological Science.

During the exposure phase, participants who saw flurps and jalets during the game had scores that were similar to those exposed to creatures with random combinations of body parts. This showed that they weren't recognizing the categories and using them to improve their response times.

However, when it came to explicit learning, prior exposure mattered. Participants exposed to the dense categories learned faster when explicitly taught about them. Exposure to the sparse categories or unrelated creatures didn't result in faster learning later.

The team repeated the experiments in different ways, making it harder to focus on the categories during the implicit exposure phase. For example, in one they had volunteers focus on a sound during the game instead of a picture.

The researchers found that prior exposure to the dense categories still primed the brain for learning. This study is one of the first to show that people learn from incidental exposure to new things.

-by Sharon Reynolds

For further information on this and other health topics, visit the web site of the National Institute of Health at www.nih.gov.

Health Talk Archives...

Sponsored by:

brooksidead2.JPG - 15389 Bytes
cvhcad2.JPG - 13883 Bytes

Window on the Clearwater
P.O. Box 2444
Orofino, ID 83544

Telephone:
Orofino 476-0733
Fax: 208-476-4140

Email